The unresponsiveness of the full-term newborn is sometimes attributed to asphyxia, even when no severe physiologic disturbance occurred during labor and delivery. The controversy about whether to use the name “hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy” or “newborn encephalopathy” has recently flared in publications directed toward pediatricians and neurologists. In this clinic opinion piece, I discuss the importance to obstetricians of this decision and explain why “newborn encephalopathy” should be the default term.
A full term newborn enters the world unresponsive to stimuli. Should the diagnosis be “hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy” (HIE) or “newborn encephalopathy” (NE)? Although the controversy about which label to use has been going on for more than 2 decades, it has escalated recently in the pediatric and neurology literatures. I wrote this opinion piece to bring the controversy to the obstetric literature where it belongs. The diagnostic labels you and the neonatologist apply to each child have potential consequences for each of you, the family, the hospital, and its employees.
The voice of a distinguished author can carry inappropriate weight, and thereby influence those who read, “I believe that we have an obligation to provide the most accurate information that we can in a given infant concerning the nature and extent of the neuropathology, the likely cause, the most probable outcome, and the best available therapy. Thus, I feel strongly that neonatal HIE, and not a vague designation (NE) is the appropriate terminology for the encephalopathy seen in term infants.
This clinical opinion essay provides another perspective. The diagnosis of HIE is appropriate when the cerebral blood flow is documented to be sufficiently reduced, the oxygen content of the blood delivered to the brain is below a level needed to avoid energy failure in brain cells, and brain metabolism is so low that cell integrity cannot be maintained. However, such assessments of the fetus or the newborn are not yet routinely available.
These limitations have prompted a reliance on biomarkers of exposure to presumed hypoxia and ischemia near the time of birth. Unfortunately, they do less well than desired differentiating term infants who develop NE from those who do not. For example, only about one quarter of children who had a base excess of −10 mml/L. Thirty to 45 minutes after birth fulfilled criteria for NE. A base excess of −14 mmol/L, however, had a sensitivity of 73% and a specificity of 82% for identifying NE.
Despite the hope that electronic fetal monitoring would help reduce the occurrence of cerebral palsy, identify newborns whose base excess has surpassed an unacceptable level (eg, >−12 mmol/L), or provide knowledge of the duration, mechanism, and severity of hypoxia and occasionally, the timing of neurologic injury, these hopes have yet to be realized. Cardiotocographic abnormalities (including bradycardia, decreased variability, nonreactivity, and variable decelerations) are not good predictors of severe metabolic acidosis. Even advances in cardiotocography with ST waveform analysis do not appear to predict NE or metabolic acidosis at birth.
In the presence of a nonreassuring fetal heart rate pattern, fetal scalp lactate sampling does not appear to improve the clinician’s ability to reduce the occurrence of NE. This observation leads to the inference that the clinician’s response to fetal acidemia does not minimize brain damage.
In light of these observations, the only surrogate of cerebral oxygen use that currently provides some discriminating information is an extremely low base excess . Severely low base excess, however, should be seen as having its own set of antecedents.
I address only 5 points, what constitutes a cause, the evidence that hypoxic-ischemic exposures contribute to the newborn’s unresponsiveness, the evidence that other characteristics and exposures contribute to this situation, how attributing an unfortunate occurrence to a proceeding event, characteristic, or exposure is fraught with biases, and the consequences of using one diagnostic label or another.
What constitutes a cause?
Epidemiologists continue to argue about what constitutes a cause. The general consensus, however, is that single-cause attribution is almost never correct, and that variants of the multiple causation model are likely to be most helpful. Even the current proponent of HIE accepts the concept of multiple causation models.
I am not aware of epidemiologists or thoughtful biologists who accept a deterministic model of disease causation that implies if A occurs, then B will invariably follow. Rather, a probabilistic model is the norm, which has the characteristic that if A occurs, then the risk of B is increased. The sufficient component view of disease causation holds that a disease will occur only when sufficiently many of the contributions are present.
Epidemiologists who conduct observational studies tend to be wary of implying causation. Rather, we tend to talk about associations and contributors to models of disease occurrence. Universal acceptance of a set of criteria for what constitutes a cause has yet to be achieved.
These causal inference limitations have not dissuaded task forces of distinguished organizations from trying to establish causal relationships between sets of delivery and newborn characteristics and brain damage in the newborn. The results of these attempts, however, have been less than satisfying. For example, among neonates with a sentinal event characterized by a sudden prolonged fetal heart rate deceleration that lasted until delivery, only 10% of the neonates demonstrated all 4 of the criteria offered by the American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology task force to relate perinatal events to the subsequent development of cerebral palsy. Only 2 of 46 children born at term who developed cerebral palsy satisfied criteria established by the American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists together with the American Academy of Pediatrics to identify acute intrapartum hypoxia.
The worth of the causal criteria tabulated by authoritative task forces remains to be established. Limitations of the latest task force’s recommendations prompted the creation of what was hoped would be an improved classification of cardiotocographic abnormalities. Nevertheless, even these efforts have been challenged. An assessment of how well the proposed graded classification of fetal heart rate tracings predicted neonatal metabolic acidosis and NE is limited by the addition of medicolegal cases to an unselected sample.
In the absence of universally accepted guidelines, the extent of the encephalopathy should be described, and consideration given to hypothermia and other therapies intended to minimize the extent of brain damage.
Multihit models of brain damage
Cancer epidemiology benefited considerably from the concept of what was at first a 2-hit model of cancer biology, and extended to become the multihit model. In these models, no single exposure, results in damage. Rather, multiple exposures, each at an intensity incapable of causing damage by itself, are needed to result in any damage. To some extent, the multiple-cause model of brain damage in the newborn is just this kind of multihit model with no single exposure sufficient to result in appreciable damage.
“Sensitization” is the name given to the phenomenon of one low-intensity (ie, subdamaging) exposure allowing a subsequent subdamaging exposure to result in damage. For example, in 7-day old rats, short periods of hypoxia—ischemia that by themselves cause no or little injury, will result in obvious injury if the animals were given a low dose of intraperitoneal endotoxin 4 hours or 72 hours earlier. This phenomenon of preceding inflammation increasing the brain damage caused by subsequent damage-promoting exposures is supported by other studies.
In the human born at term, the multihit model appears to apply to the risk of NE, as well as retinopathy of prematurity. The multihit model might also apply to brain damage in the preterm human newborn, but it is not yet clear if the potentially damaging exposures are prolonged or recurrent. The obvious inference is that in some situations, multiple exposures are needed to result in damage.
Multihit models of brain damage
Cancer epidemiology benefited considerably from the concept of what was at first a 2-hit model of cancer biology, and extended to become the multihit model. In these models, no single exposure, results in damage. Rather, multiple exposures, each at an intensity incapable of causing damage by itself, are needed to result in any damage. To some extent, the multiple-cause model of brain damage in the newborn is just this kind of multihit model with no single exposure sufficient to result in appreciable damage.
“Sensitization” is the name given to the phenomenon of one low-intensity (ie, subdamaging) exposure allowing a subsequent subdamaging exposure to result in damage. For example, in 7-day old rats, short periods of hypoxia—ischemia that by themselves cause no or little injury, will result in obvious injury if the animals were given a low dose of intraperitoneal endotoxin 4 hours or 72 hours earlier. This phenomenon of preceding inflammation increasing the brain damage caused by subsequent damage-promoting exposures is supported by other studies.
In the human born at term, the multihit model appears to apply to the risk of NE, as well as retinopathy of prematurity. The multihit model might also apply to brain damage in the preterm human newborn, but it is not yet clear if the potentially damaging exposures are prolonged or recurrent. The obvious inference is that in some situations, multiple exposures are needed to result in damage.
“Causes” of unresponsiveness at birth
When many of us use the term “newborn encephalopathy,” we make no assumption about a hypoxic-ischemic etiology. The assumption that only intrapartum hypoxia-ischemia causes an infant to be limp and unresponsive at birth is not supported by epidemiologic studies, which show that increased risk of NE is associated with a variety of maternal characteristics (low hemoglobin, low thyroxine concentrations, fever during labor), as well as fetal characteristics (severe growth restriction, persistent occiput-posterior position), and placenta lesions. Because other phenomena can contribute to the risk of NE, a single-attribution name should not be applied to an entity that probably has many “causes.”
A variety of gene mutations and congenital malformations of the brain in newborn rodents and humans have been associated with hypoventilation, frequent apneic episodes, and failure to increase breathing in response to hypoxemia and/or hypercarbia. For example, a retrospective chart review of 48 individuals with Prader-Willi syndrome, 23% had asphyxia at birth, compared with an expected rate of 1%.
Although some of these disorders first present during, or even after childhood, phenotypic variability can be prominent. Indeed, these disorders have the potential to present with a picture indistinguishable from NE.
Acidemia, sometimes considered an objective biomarker of asphyxia, can be a reflection of metabolic disorders that are not a consequence of hypoxemia. Nevertheless, some people have claimed to know when ischemia and hypoxemia occurred in unresponsive newborns. Others of us recognize our limitations.
Our hesitancy is supported by the finding that after excluding newborns with chromosomal abnormalities and congenital malformations, the brain damage in term infants who appeared asphyxiated and encephalopathic before death at least 3 days after birth was consistent with onset before the start of labor. One inference that follows from this is that an intrapartum sentinel event might not provide information about when the brain damage was initiated.
Perhaps biomarkers indicative of the intra-partum processes leading to brain damage might help distinguish evolving brain damage from other disorders with an encephalopathic phenotype. We are not there yet.
Evidence that intrapartum hypoxic-ischemic exposures result in an encephalopathy
Much of the argument for the use of the term “hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy” is based on experimental models that asphyxiate a presumably normal young animal and argue on that basis to the situation in human infants. A number of students of animal models are not sure how well animal models in general match the human condition. Thus, it seems inappropriate to put much emphasis on analogies to laboratory models.
The argument that morphologic lesions of the brain indicate etiology is also open to question. This statement applies to the multiple histologic and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) lesions attributed to hypoxia-ischemia. Even sentinel events are not followed by a unique MRI signature. One group of investigators wrote, “CT and MRI documentation of basal ganglia injury are associated with metabolic acidemia and HIE (hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy), but are not precise enough to serve as a gold standard in the identification of these conditions.”
Attribution
“The human mind is designed to find reasons for … things rather than attribute them to chance.” Attribution psychology deals with the processes people use to infer the causes of behaviors and events. So how should one decide whether HIE is the best diagnosis?
It is not easy. Even those considered most knowledgeable are prone to bias in offering causal explanations for undesirable events. Decision makers tend to focus on what is available and what is perceived as salient, although ignoring other potentially important considerations. This can be especially important when unidentified intrauterine exposures can influence the newborn’s physiology, even as long as weeks later.
In the absence of a sentinel event such as cord prolapse or amniotic fluid embolism, most clinicians are highly unlikely to be able to identify THE cause of the newborn’s unresponsiveness. As an epidemiologist and neurologist with a special interest in the antecedents of perinatal brain damage and more than 40 years of experience in the field, I still do not feel comfortable doing so.
Consequences of using one name or another
Close the door and shutter the windows and your wider vision is limited. Use the term HIE and your inclination to search for alternative explanations for flaccidity and unresponsiveness might be diminished.
Malpractice claims against obstetricians drastically increase costs of medical malpractice insurance, prompt insurers to withdraw from the market, and fail to provide compensation for most congenitally impaired children. Malpractice claims against obstetricians also lead to great stress. To eliminate these problems, no-fault cerebral palsy insurance was first suggested more than 20 years ago. Subsequently, Virginia and Florida created funds and no-fault mechanisms to compensate families for the lifetime medical expenses they incur when their child is born severely impaired because of neurologic injuries thought to have been sustained during the birth process. This legislation along with legislation to cap awards appears to have made affordable malpractice insurance available to obstetricians in these states. On the other hand, other benefits are less clear.
In the meantime, the vast majority of states do not have compensation programs for families of children born with severe neurologic impairments. Consequently, many of the undesirable attributes of the malpractice system continue. Until they are eliminated, any potential to obfuscate what did, and did not, contribute to the child’s impairments should be avoided, including the use of terms (eg, “HIE”) that allow the plaintiff’s bar to use counterfactuals (“if only the obstetrician had … , this disaster would have been avoided”).
How much of NE is preventable remains unknown. Perhaps the best way to reduce the occurrence of preventable encephalopathy (which is yet to be defined) is not through litigation, but through quality improvement efforts.
Conclusion
When many of the causes of an important clinical syndrome are not known, terminology should not be used that puts off inquiry. Applying one etiologic term to neonatal depression/unresponsiveness, regardless of what the causes might be, is likely to limit the search for gene mutations, as well as congenital structural or functional brain lesions that are present at the time of delivery. Applying one etiologic term to a limp child after a presumed compromising event also poses inferential problems. Caution is always appropriate in identifying THE CAUSE of the newborn’s dysfunctions. Perhaps the most reasonable solution then is to have “NE” as the default diagnosis.
The author reports no conflict of interest.
Reprints not available from the author.
References
- 1. Nelson K.B., and Leviton A.: How much of neonatal encephalopathy is due to birth asphyxia? Am J Dis Child 1991; 145: pp. 1325-1331
- 2. Dammann O., Ferriero D., and Gressens P.: Neonatal encephalopathy or hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy? Pediatr Res 2011; 70: pp. 1-2
- 3. Volpe J.J.: Neonatal encephalopathy—an inadequate term for hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Ann Neurol 2012; 72: pp. 156-166
- 4. Bennet L., Booth L., and Gunn A.J.: Potential biomarkers for hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 2010; 15: pp. 253-260
- 5. Wayenberg J.L.: Threshold of metabolic acidosis associated with neonatal encephalopathy in the term newborn. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2005; 18: pp. 381-385
- 6. Quilligan E.J., and Paul R.H.: Fetal monitoring: is it worth it? Obstet Gynecol 1975; 45: pp. 96-100
- 7. Ross M.G.: Labor and fetal heart rate decelerations: relation to fetal metabolic acidosis. Clin Obstet Gynecol 2011; 54: pp. 74-82
- 8. Schifrin B.S., and Ater S.: Fetal hypoxic and ischemic injuries. Curr Opin Obstet Gynecol 2006; 18: pp. 112-122
- 9. Larma J.D., Silva A.M., Holcroft C.J., Thompson R.E., Donohue P.K., and Graham E.M.: Intrapartum electronic fetal heart rate monitoring and the identification of metabolic acidosis and hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2007; 197: pp. 301.e1-301.e8
- 10. Neilson J.P.: Fetal electrocardiogram (ECG) for fetal monitoring during labour. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2012; 4:
- 11. Potti S., and Berghella V.: ST waveform analysis versus cardiotocography alone for intrapartum fetal monitoring: a meta-analysis of randomized trials. Am J Perinatol 2012; 29: pp. 657-664
- 12. Becker J.H., Bax L., Amer-Wahlin I., et al: ST analysis of the fetal electrocardiogram in intrapartum fetal monitoring: a meta-analysis. Obstet Gynecol 2012; 119: pp. 145-154
- 13. East C.E., Leader L.R., Sheehan P., Henshall N.E., and Colditz P.B.: Intrapartum fetal scalp lactate sampling for fetal assessment in the presence of a non-reassuring fetal heart rate trace. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2010; undefined:
- 14. Locatelli A., Incerti M., Ghidini A., Greco M., Villa E., and Paterlini G.: Factors associated with umbilical artery acidemia in term infants with low Apgar scores at 5 min. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol 2008; 139: pp. 146-150
- 15. Susser M.: What is a cause and how do we know one? Am J Epidemiol 1991; 133: pp. 635-648
- 16. Parascandola M., and Weed D.L.: Causation in epidemiology. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001; 55: pp. 905-912
- 17. Campaner R.: Mechanistic causality and counterfactual-manipulative causality: recent insights from philosophy of science. J Epidemiol Community Health 2011; 65: pp. 1070-1074
- 18. Hoffmann K., Heidemann C., Weikert C., Schulze M.B., and Boeing H.: Estimating the proportion of disease due to classes of sufficient causes. Am J Epidemiol 2006; 163: pp. 76-83
- 19. Leviton A.: Single-cause attribution. Dev Med Child Neurol 1987; 29: pp. 805-807
- 20. Bunin G.: What causes childhood brain tumors? Pediatr Neurosurg 2000; 32: pp. 321-326
- 21. Brown R.L.: Epidemiology of injury and the impact of health disparities. Curr Opin Pediatr 2010; 22: pp. 321-325
- 22. Cederberg D., and Siesjo P.: What has inflammation to do with traumatic brain injury? Childs Nerv Syst 2010; 26: pp. 221-226
- 23. Thornton C., Rousset C.I., Kichev A., et al: Molecular mechanisms of neonatal brain injury. Neurol Res Int 2012; 2012: pp. 506320
- 24. Limperopoulos C., Benson C.B., Bassan H., et al: Cerebellar hemorrhage in the preterm infant: ultrasonographic findings and risk factors. Pediatrics 2005; 116: pp. 717-724
- 25. Mayr E.: Cause and effect in biology. Science 1961; 134: pp. 1501-1506
- 26. Laland K.N., Sterelny K., Odling-Smee J., Hoppitt W., and Uller T.: Cause and effect in biology revisited: is Mayr’s proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science 2011; 334: pp. 1512-1516
- 27. Dammann O., and Leviton A.: Perinatal brain damage causation. Dev Neurosci 2007; 29: pp. 280-288
- 28. Rothman K.J.: Causes. Am J Epidemiol 1976; 104: pp. 587-592
- 29. Morabia A.: On the origin of Hill’s causal criteria. Epidemiology 1991; 2: pp. 367-369
- 30. Weed D.L.: Epidemiologic evidence and causal inference. Hematol Oncol Clin North Am 2000; 14: pp. 797-807
- 31. Ward A.C.: The role of causal criteria in causal inferences: Bradford Hill’s “aspects of association”. Epidemiol Perspect Innov 2009; 6: pp. 2
- 32. Fetal and neonatal neurologic injury. Washington, DC: American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists, 1992.
- 33. MacLennan A.: A template for defining a causal relation between acute intrapartum events and cerebral palsy: international consensus statement. BMJ 1999; 319: pp. 1054-1059
- 34. Neonatal encephalopathy and cerebral palsy: defining the pathogenesis and pathophysiology.
- 35. Korst L.M., Phelan J.P., Wang Y.M., Martin G.I., and Ahn M.O.: Acute fetal asphyxia and permanent brain injury: a retrospective analysis of current indicators. J Matern Fetal Med 1999; 8: pp. 101-106
- 36. Strijbis E.M., Oudman I., van Essen P., and MacLennan A.H.: Cerebral palsy and the application of the international criteria for acute intrapartum hypoxia. Obstet Gynecol 2006; 107: pp. 1357-1365
- 37. Practice bulletin no. 116: Management of intrapartum fetal heart rate tracings. Obstet Gynecol 2010; 116: pp. 1232-1240
- 38. Freeman R.K., and Nageotte M.P.: Comments on American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists Practice Bulletin no. 106. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2010; 202: pp. 411-412
- 39. Elliott C., Warrick P.A., Graham E., and Hamilton E.F.: Graded classification of fetal heart rate tracings: association with neonatal metabolic acidosis and neurologic morbidity. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2010; 202: pp. 258.e1-258.e8
- 40. Leviton A., and Nelson K.B.: Problems with definitions and classifications of newborn encephalopathy. Pediatr Neurol 1992; 8: pp. 85-90
- 41. Higgins R.D., Raju T., Edwards A.D., et al: Hypothermia and other treatment options for neonatal encephalopathy: an executive summary of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHD workshop. J Pediatr 2011; 159: pp. 851-858.e1
- 42. Ashley D.J.: The two “hit” and multiple “hit” theories of carcinogenesis. Br J Cancer 1969; 23: pp. 313-328
- 43. Knudson A.G.: Mutation and cancer: statistical study of retinoblastoma. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1971; 68: pp. 820-823
- 44. Freeman R.K.: Medical and legal implications for necessary requirements to diagnose damaging hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy leading to later cerebral palsy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2008; 199: pp. 585-586
- 45. Eklind S., Mallard C., Leverin A.L., et al: Bacterial endotoxin sensitizes the immature brain to hypoxic-ischaemic injury. Eur J Neurosci 2001; 13: pp. 1101-1106
- 46. Eklind S., Mallard C., Arvidsson P., and Hagberg H.: Lipopolysaccharide induces both a primary and a secondary phase of sensitization in the developing rat brain. Pediatr Res 2005; 58: pp. 112-116
- 47. Kim Y., Busto R., Dietrich W.D., Kraydieh S., and Ginsberg M.D.: Delayed postischemic hyperthermia in awake rats worsens the histopathological outcome of transient focal cerebral ischemia. Stroke 1996; 27: pp. 2274-2280
- 48. Aden U., Favrais G., Plaisant F., et al: Systemic inflammation sensitizes the neonatal brain to excitotoxicity through a pro-/anti-inflammatory imbalance: key role of TNFalpha pathway and protection by etanercept. Brain Behav Immun 2010; 24: pp. 747-758
- 49. Wang X., Stridh L., Li W., et al: Lipopolysaccharide sensitizes neonatal hypoxic-ischemic brain injury in a MyD88-dependent manner. J Immunol 2009; 183: pp. 7471-7477
- 50. Markus T., Cronberg T., Cilio C., Pronk C., Wieloch T., and Ley D.: Tumor necrosis factor receptor-1 is essential for LPS-induced sensitization and tolerance to oxygen-glucose deprivation in murine neonatal organotypic hippocampal slices. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2009; 29: pp. 73-86
- 51. McColl B.W., Allan S.M., and Rothwell N.J.: Systemic infection, inflammation and acute ischemic stroke. Neuroscience 2009; 158: pp. 1049-1061
- 52. Kendall G.S., Hirstova M., Horn S., et al: TNF gene cluster deletion abolishes lipopolysaccharide-mediated sensitization of the neonatal brain to hypoxic ischemic insult. Lab Invest 2011; 91: pp. 328-341
- 53. Tomimatsu T., Fukuda H., Kanagawa T., Mu J., Kanzaki T., and Murata Y.: Effects of hyperthermia on hypoxic-ischemic brain damage in the immature rat: its influence on caspase-3-like protease. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2003; 188: pp. 768-773
- 54. Impey L.W., Greenwood C.E., Black R.S., Yeh P.S., Sheil O., and Doyle P.: The relationship between intrapartum maternal fever and neonatal acidosis as risk factors for neonatal encephalopathy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2008; 198: pp. 49.e1-49.e6
- 55. Adamson S.J., Alessandri L.M., Badawi N., Burton P.R., Pemberton P.J., and Stanley F.: Predictors of neonatal encephalopathy in full-term infants. BMJ 1995; 311: pp. 598-602
- 56. Badawi N., Kurinczuk J.J., Keogh J.M., et al: Intrapartum risk factors for newborn encephalopathy: the Western Australian case-control study. BMJ 1998; 317: pp. 1554-1558
- 57. Dammann O., Brinkhaus M.J., Bartels D.B., et al: Immaturity, perinatal inflammation, and retinopathy of prematurity: a multi-hit hypothesis. Early Hum Dev 2009; 85: pp. 325-329
- 58. Leviton A., Kuban K., Allred E.N., Fichorova R., O’Shea T.M., and Paneth N.: Elevated concentrations of circulating indicators of inflammation in infants born before the 28th post-menstrual week predict a small head circumference two years later. Early Hum Dev 2011; 87: pp. 325-330
- 59. Leviton A., Kuban K., O’Shea T.M., et al: The relationship between early concentrations of 25 blood proteins and cerebral white matter injury in preterm newborns: the ELGAN study. J Pediatr 2011; 158: pp. 897-903.e5
- 60. O’Shea T.M., Allred E.N., Kuban K., et al: Elevated concentrations of inflammation-related proteins in postnatal blood predict severe developmental delay at two years in extremely premature infants. J Pediatr 2012; 160: pp. 395-401.e4
- 61. Bilbo S.D., Biedenkapp J.C., Der-Avakian A., Watkins L.R., Rudy J.W., and Maier S.F.: Neonatal infection-induced memory impairment after lipopolysaccharide in adulthood is prevented via caspase-1 inhibition. J Neurosci 2005; 25: pp. 8000-8009
- 62. Kaindl A.M., Favrais G., and Gressens P.: Molecular mechanisms involved in injury to the preterm brain. J Child Neurol 2009; 24: pp. 1112-1118
- 63. Kroon A.A., Wang J., Huang Z., Cao L., Kuliszewski M., and Post M.: Inflammatory response to oxygen and endotoxin in newborn rat lung ventilated with low tidal volume. Pediatr Res 2010; 68: pp. 63-69
- 64. Orman M.A., Ierapetritou M.G., Berthiaume F., and Androulakis I.P.: The dynamics of the early inflammatory response in double-hit burn and sepsis animal models. Cytokine 2011; 56: pp. 494-502
- 65. Williamson L.L., Sholar P.W., Mistry R.S., Smith S.H., and Bilbo S.D.: Microglia and memory: modulation by early-life infection. J Neurosci 2011; 31: pp. 15511-15521
- 66. Nelson K.B., and Chang T.: Is cerebral palsy preventable? Curr Opin Neurol 2008; 21: pp. 129-135
- 67. Nelson K.B.: Causative factors in cerebral palsy. Clin Obstet Gynecol 2008; 51: pp. 749-762
- 68. Finer N.N., Robertson C.M., Richards R.T., Pinnell L.E., and Peters K.L.: Hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy in term neonates: perinatal factors and outcome. J Pediatr 1981; 98: pp. 112-117
- 69. Levene M.L., Kornberg J., and Williams T.H.: The incidence and severity of post-asphyxial encephalopathy in full-term infants. Early Hum Dev 1985; 11: pp. 21-26
- 70. Airede A.I.: Birth asphyxia and hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy: incidence and severity. Ann Trop Paediatr 1991; 11: pp. 331-335
- 71. Badawi N., Kurinczuk J.J., Keogh J.M., et al: Antepartum risk factors for newborn encephalopathy: the Western Australian case-control study. BMJ 1998; 317: pp. 1549-1553
- 72. Berg A.T.: Indices of fetal growth-retardation, perinatal hypoxia-related factors and childhood neurological morbidity. Early Hum Dev 1989; 19: pp. 271-283
- 73. Ellis M., Manandhar N., Manandhar D.S., and Costello A.M.: Risk factors for neonatal encephalopathy in Kathmandu, Nepal, a developing country: unmatched case-control study. BMJ 2000; 320: pp. 1229-1236
- 74. Wintermark P., Boyd T., Gregas M.C., Labrecque M., and Hansen A.: Placental pathology in asphyxiated newborns meeting the criteria for therapeutic hypothermia. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2010; 203: pp. 579.e1-579.e9
- 75. Wharton R.H., and Bresnan M.J.: Neonatal respiratory depression and delay in diagnosis in Prader-Willi syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol 1989; 31: pp. 231-236
- 76. Nsegbe E., Wallen-Mackenzie A., Dauger S., et al: Congenital hypoventilation and impaired hypoxic response in Nurr1 mutant mice. J Physiol 2004; 556: pp. 43-59
- 77. Berner J., Shvarev Y., Lagercrantz H., Bilkei-Gorzo A., Hokfelt T., and Wickstrom R.: Altered respiratory pattern and hypoxic response in transgenic newborn mice lacking the tachykinin-1 gene. J Appl Physiol 2007; 103: pp. 552-559
- 78. Tomycz N.D., Haynes R.L., Schmidt E.F., Ackerson K., and Kinney H.C.: Novel neuropathologic findings in the Haddad syndrome. Acta Neuropathol 2010; 119: pp. 261-269
- 79. Armangue T., Macaya A., Vazquez E., Jurado M.J., and Roig-Quilis M.: Central hypoventilation and brainstem dysgenesis. Pediatr Neurol 2012; 46: pp. 257-259
- 80. Jennings L.J., Yu M., Rand C.M., et al: Variable human phenotype associated with novel deletions of the PHOX2B gene. Pediatr Pulmonol 2012; 47: pp. 153-161
- 81. Schule B., Armstrong D.D., Vogel H., Oviedo A., and Francke U.: Severe congenital encephalopathy caused by MECP2 null mutations in males: central hypoxia and reduced neuronal dendritic structure. Clin Genet 2008; 74: pp. 116-126
- 82. Graham E.M., Ruis K.A., Hartman A.L., Northington F.J., and Fox H.E.: A systematic review of the role of intrapartum hypoxia-ischemia in the causation of neonatal encephalopathy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2008; 199: pp. 587-595
- 83. Shalak L., Johnson-Welch S., and Perlman J.M.: Chorioamnionitis and neonatal encephalopathy in term infants with fetal acidemia: histopathologic correlations. Pediatr Neurol 2005; 33: pp. 162-165
- 84. Depp R.: Perinatal asphyxia: assessing its causal role and timing. Semin Pediatr Neurol 1995; 2: pp. 3-36
- 85. Naeye R.L., and Localio A.R.: Determining the time before birth when ischemia and hypoxemia initiated cerebral palsy. Obstet Gynecol 1995; 86: pp. 713-719
- 86. Draper E.S., Kurinczuk J.J., Lamming C.R., Clarke M., James D., and Field D.: A confidential enquiry into cases of neonatal encephalopathy. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2002; 87: pp. F176-F180
- 87. Cowan F., Rutherford M., Groenendaal F., et al: Origin and timing of brain lesions in term infants with neonatal encephalopathy. Lancet 2003; 361: pp. 736-742
- 88. Becher J.C., Bell J.E., Keeling J.W., McIntosh N., and Wyatt B.: The Scottish perinatal neuropathology study: clinicopathological correlation in early neonatal deaths. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2004; 89: pp. F399-F407
- 89. Ennen C.S., Huisman T.A., Savage W.J., et al: Glial fibrillary acidic protein as a biomarker for neonatal hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy treated with whole-body cooling. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2011; 205: pp. 251.e1-251.e7
- 90. Lynch V.J.: Use with caution: developmental systems divergence and potential pitfalls of animal models. Yale J Biol Med 2009; 82: pp. 53-66
- 91. van der Worp H.B., Howells D.W., Sena E.S., et al: Can animal models of disease reliably inform human studies? PLoS Med 2010; 7: pp. e1000245
- 92. Regenberg A., Mathews D.J., Blass D.M., et al: The role of animal models in evaluating reasonable safety and efficacy for human trials of cell-based interventions for neurologic conditions. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2009; 29: pp. 1-9
- 93. Perlman J.M.: Intrapartum hypoxic-ischemic cerebral injury and subsequent cerebral palsy: medicolegal issues. Pediatrics 1997; 99: pp. 851-859
- 94. Gilles F.H., and Gomez I.G.: Developmental neuropathology of the second half of gestation. Early Hum Dev 2005; 81: pp. 245-253
- 95. Shroff M.M., Soares-Fernandes J.P., Whyte H., and Raybaud C.: MR imaging for diagnostic evaluation of encephalopathy in the newborn. Radiographics 2010; 30: pp. 763-780
- 96. Okereafor A., Allsop J., Counsell S.J., et al: Patterns of brain injury in neonates exposed to perinatal sentinel events. Pediatrics 2008; 121: pp. 906-914
- 97. Ruis K.A., Lehmann C.U., Northington F.J., Lin D.D., and Graham E.M.: Neonatal brain imaging and the identification of metabolic acidemia and hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2009; 22: pp. 823-828
- 98. Ankrum N.: Readings. Austin, TX: Austin Chronicle, 2008.
- 99. Gyekye S.A.: Occupational safety management: the role of causal attribution. Int J Psychol 2010; 45: pp. 405-416
- 100. Tversky A., and Kahneman D.: Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogn Psychol 1973; 5: pp. 207-232
- 101. Spellman B.A., Kincannon A., and Stose S.: The relation between counterfactual and causal reasoning. In Mandel D.R., Hilton D.J., and Catellani P. (eds): The psychology of counterfactual thinking. London: Routledge, 2005.
- 102. Wegwarth O., Gaissmaier W., and Gigerenzer G.: Smart strategies for doctors and doctors-in-training: heuristics in medicine. Med Educ 2009; 43: pp. 721-728
- 103. Gerstenberg T., and Lagnado D.A.: Spreading the blame: the allocation of responsibility amongst multiple agents. Cognition 2010; 115: pp. 166-171
- 104. Gerstenberg T., and Lagnado D.A.: When contributions make a difference: explaining order effects in responsibility attribution. Psychon Bull Rev 2012; 19: pp. 729-736
- 105. Lee A.J., Lambermont V.A., Pillow J.J., et al: Fetal responses to lipopolysaccharide-induced chorioamnionitis alter immune and airway responses in 7-week-old sheep. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2011; 204: pp. 364.e17-364.e24
- 106. Freeman A.D., and Freeman J.M.: No-fault cerebral palsy insurance: an alternative to the obstetrical malpractice lottery. J Health Polit Policy Law 1989; 14: pp. 707-718
- 107. Committee opinion no. 497: Coping with the stress of medical professional liability litigation. Obstet Gynecol 2011; 118: pp. 389-391
- 108. Review of the Virginia birth-related neurological injury. Compensation Program 2002; 2002:
- 109. Spigel S.: Virginia and Florida child brain injury compensation funds. Report to the Connecticut Office of Legislative Research.
- 110. Clark S.L., Belfort M.A., Byrum S.L., Meyers J.A., and Perlin J.B.: Improved outcomes, fewer cesarean deliveries, and reduced litigation: results of a new paradigm in patient safety. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2008; 199: pp. 105.e1-105.e7