Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of permanent developmental disabilities characterized by abnormalities in socialization, communication, and behavior. ASDs are classified under pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs) and include autistic disorder (AD), Asperger’s syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS), which is also referred to as atypical autism.
The diagnosis of ASDs is usually made in early childhood, and the condition often leads to a compromise in the quality of life for both the affected child and the entire family. ASDs affect male more than female children on a 4:1 ratio. There has been a temporal increase in the prevalence of autism, and it is currently estimated that approximately 1 in 88 children in the United States suffer from ASDs. The rise has been attributed to increased awareness and changes in administrative classifications and definitions as well as to changes in practice patterns leading to improved diagnosis of the condition. Although a genetic predisposition to this condition has been firmly established, the exact etiology remains unknown.
Research has shown that environmental factors may play a role including perinatal risk factors. Studies have shown that preterm birth, smallness for gestational age, congenital anomalies, low Apgar scores, advanced maternal age, maternal metabolic syndrome, smoking, and exposure to betamimetic agents during pregnancy may play a role, but there is insufficient evidence to causally implicate any of these factors. In contrast, maternal hypertension, proteinuria, and preeclampsia may be protective for ASD.
Oxytocin is a neurotransmitter with important central nervous system effects regarding reproduction, social behavior, and cognitive function. Because of its possible influences in social behavior and cognitive function, oxytocin has been linked to triggers of ASDs at the molecular level, and it has been postulated that polymorphic variation at the oxytocin receptor gene may be associated with ASDs, depending on early environmental conditions. As a result, it has been hypothesized that oxytocin administration during labor may contribute to the development of ASDs by down-regulation of the fetal brain oxytocin receptors.
Given this background, it would make perfect sense to examine the clinical association between oxytocin during labor and ASDs, especially in the setting of a rising oxytocin use during labor.
Indeed, in a recent publication, Gregory et al reported for the first time an epidemiological study that examined the association between induced or augmented labor and the risk of ASDs. The authors linked 2 databases: the North Carolina Detailed Birth Records for births that occurred during the years 1990-1998 and the North Carolina Education Research data to identify children diagnosed with autism during the years 1997-2007. The authors concluded that induction or augmentation during childbirth is associated with increased odds of autism diagnosis in childhood and that the observed associations were particularly pronounced in male children. The conclusion of the study immediately gained widespread attention by news media.
Because this is an extremely important issue for obstetricians and patients, the remainder of this editorial will be focused on some important issues related to the primary exposure and interpretation of the results. We will offer an alternative interpretation of the study findings.
It has been previously hypothesized that a predisposing factor for autism may be oxytocin exposure. However, the primary exposure in the study by Gregory et al was not oxytocin exposure but “augmentation or induction of labor” as a proxy for oxytocin exposure. Because of the limitations of the data, the authors were unable to separate those patients who had exposure only to oxytocin. Prior and during the study birth years (1990-1998), an unknown mixture of agents of various dosages had been used for labor induction including amniotomy, various prostaglandins, and intracervical balloon catheters. These agents could have been used alone or in addition to oxytocin. Thus, the primary exposure is not the desired agent (oxytocin).
It is fundamental for all observational comparative studies to examine and report any known practice changes that could have influenced the results of the study. Unfortunately, there were significant changes in the criteria for diagnosing PDD-NOS (atypical autism) during the study years. The American Psychiatry Association reported that an editorial error was made in the description of the diagnostic criteria for PDD-NOS category of Autism Spectrum Disorders in the 1994 publication of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , fourth edition (DSM-IV). Instead of requiring “impairment in social interaction and in verbal or nonverbal communication skills,” DSM-IV stated–because of the editorial error–that the “PDD-NOS category should be used when there is a severe and pervasive impairment of reciprocal social interaction or verbal and nonverbal communication skills or when stereotyped behavior, interests, and activities are present.”
In 2000, research showed that there had been overdiagnosis of PDD-NOS in the prior years, and therefore, the wording of the criteria for PDD-NOS were revised back to the original definition in the DSM-IV, Text Revision, which was published in May 2000. Because the wrong wording of DSM-IV led to the overdiagnosis of PDD-NOS prior to year 2000, this most likely led to misclassification of ASD in the study by Gregory et al.
Because the autism diagnoses were made between 1997 and 2007, it is certain that there were overdiagnoses in the initial years of the study. This overdiagnosis of ASDs may well explain the positive associations between augmentation and/or induction and autism during the initial birth years 1990-1995. Because the median age at diagnosis of PDD-NOS in North Carolina was 55 months and the correct diagnostic criteria for PDD-NOS were applied from 2000 onward, it is expected that the correct criteria were applied only to those who were born 4-5 years earlier and onward (ie, 1996-1998). However, there were no statistically significant associations between induction/augmentation during labor and autism in years 1996-1998 in comparison to 1990 (reference year).
A careful inspection of the yearly trend of the associations between augmentation and/or induction and autism (Table 3 in Gregory et al ) reveals that there was a sudden change to nonsignificant associations during each of the 3 most recent birth years of the study (1996-1998) when the correct diagnostic criteria for autistic disorders were applied (compared to 1990).
We believe that the conclusion by Gregory et al should have been that the most recent evidence (birth years 1995-1998) indicates that when the appropriate diagnostic criteria for autistic disorders are applied, there was no increase in autism from baseline or any association between induced or augmented childbirth and autism. Therefore, based on the existing data so far, we believe that there is no evidence that oxytocin administration for labor augmentation or induction or labor augmentation or induction in general increase the risk for autism.
References
- 1. Pervasive developmental disorders. In (eds): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000. pp. 69-70
- 2. Volkmar F.R., Szatmari P., and Sparrow S.S.: Sex differences in pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1993; 23: pp. 579-591
- 3. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. MMWR Surveill Summ 2012; 61: pp. 1-19
- 4. Levy S.E., Mandell D.S., and Schultz R.T.: Autism. Lancet 2009; 374: pp. 1627-1638
- 5. Ozonoff S., Young G.S., Carter A., et al: Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders; a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics 2011; 128: pp. e488-e495
- 6. Moore G.S., Kneitel A.W., Walker C.K., Gilbert W.M., and Xing G.: Autism risk in small- and large-for-gestational-age infants. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2012; 206: pp. 314.e1-314.e9
- 7. Hultman C.M., Sparen P., and Cnattingius S.: Perinatal risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology 2002; 13: pp. 417-423
- 8. Bilder D., Pinborough-Zimmerman J., Miller J., and McMahon W.: Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors associated with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2009; 123: pp. 1293-1300
- 9. Gardener H., Spiegelman D., and Buka S.L.: Prenatal risk factors for autism: comprehensive meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: pp. 7-14
- 10. Guinchat V., Thorsen P., Laurent C., Cans C., Bodeau N., and Cohen D.: Pre-, peri- and neonatal risk factors for autism. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 2012; 91: pp. 287-300
- 11. Krakowiak P., Walker C.K., Bremer A.A., et al: Maternal metabolic conditions and risk for autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Pediatrics 2012; 129: pp. e1121-e1128
- 12. Lampi K.M., Lehtonen L., Tran P.L., et al: Risk of autism spectrum disorders in low birth weight and small for gestational age infants. J Pediatr 2012; 161: pp. 830-836
- 13. Rodier P., Miller R.K., and Brent R.L.: Does treatment of premature labor with terbutaline increase the risk of autism spectrum disorders? Am J Obstet Gynecol 2011; 204: pp. 91-94
- 14. Witter F.R., Zimmerman A.W., Reichmann J.P., and Connors S.L.: In utero beta 2 adrenergic agonist exposure and adverse neurophysiologic and behavioral outcomes. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2009; 201: pp. 553-559
- 15. Insel T.R., Young L., and Wang Z.: Central oxytocin and reproductive behaviours. Rev Reprod 1997; 2: pp. 28-37
- 16. Bartz A., and Hollander E.: The neuroscience of affiliation: forging links between basic and clinical research on neuropeptides and social behavior. Horm Behav 2006; 50: pp. 518-528
- 17. Stavropoulos K.K., and Carver L.J.: Research review: social motivation and oxytocin in autism-implications for joint attention development and intervention. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013; 54: pp. 603-618
- 18. Brune M.: Does the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) polymorphism (rs2254298) confer “vulnerability” for psychopathology or differential susceptibility? Insights from evolution. BMC Med 2012; 10: pp. 38
- 19. Wahl R.U.: Could oxytocin administration during labor contribute to autism and related behavioral disorders? A look at the literature. Med Hypotheses 2004; 63: pp. 456-460
- 20. Laughton S.K., Branch D.W., Beaver J., and Zhang J.: Changes in labor patterns over 50 years. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2012; 206: pp. 419.e1-419.e9
- 21. Gregory S.G., Anthopolos R., Osgood C.E., Grotegut C.A., and Miranda M.L.: Association of autism with induced or augmented childbirth in North Carolina birth record 1990-1998 and education research (1997-2007) databases. JAMA Pediatr 2013; 167: pp. 959-966
- 22. Baxi L.V., Petrie R.H., and Caritis S.N.: Induction of labor with low-dose prostaglandin F2 alpha and oxytocin. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1980; 136: pp. 28-31
- 23. Boemi P., Reitano S., and Cianci S.: A new cervical perfusion method for induction of labor with prostaglandins. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1982; 144: pp. 476-479
- 24. MacKenzie I.Z., Bradley S., and Embrey M.P.: A simpler approach to labor induction using lipid-based prostaglandin E2 vaginal suppository. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1981; 141: pp. 158-162
- 25. Nagata I., Kato K., Makimura N., Uesato T., Seki K., and Kikuchi Y.: Comparison of plasma oxytocin levels during spontaneous labor and labor induced by amniotomy, prostaglandin F2 alpha, and prostaglandin E2. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1983; 147: pp. 259-267
- 26. Trofatter K.F., Bowers D., Gall S.A., and Killam A.P.: Preinduction cervical ripening with prostaglandin E2 (Prepidil) gel. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1985; 153: pp. 268-271
- 27. Bernstein P., Leyland N., Gurland P., and Gare D.: Cervical ripening and labor induction with prostaglandin E2 gel: a placebo-controlled study. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1987; 156: pp. 336-340
- 28. Rayburn W., Gosen R., Ramadei C., Woods R., and Scott J.: Outpatient cervical ripening with prostaglandin E2 gel in uncomplicated postdate pregnancies. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1988; 158: pp. 1417-1423
- 29. Williams J.K., Lewis M.L., Cohen G.R., and O’Brien W.F.: The sequential use of estradiol and prostaglandin E2 topical gels for cervical ripening in high-risk term pregnancies requiring induction of labor. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1988; 158: pp. 55-58
- 30. Rayburn W.F.: Prostaglandin E2 gel for cervical ripening and induction of labor: a critical analysis. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1989; 160: pp. 529-534
- 31. Smith C.V., Rayburn W.F., Connor R.E., Fredstrom G.R., and Phillips C.B.: Double-blind comparison of intravaginal prostaglandin E2 gel and “chip” for preinduction cervical ripening. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1990; 163: pp. 845-847
- 32. Jackson G.M., Sharp H.T., and Varner M.W.: Cervical ripening before induction of labor: a randomized trial of prostaglandin E2 gel versus low-dose oxytocin. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1994; 171: pp. 1092-1096
- 33. Magann E.F., Perry K.G., Dockery J.R., Bass J.D., Chauhan S.P., and Morrison J.C.: Cervical ripening before medical induction of labor: a comparison of prostaglandin E2, estradiol, andoxytocin. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1995; 172: pp. 1702-1706
- 34. Wing D.A., Jones M.M., Rahall A., Goodwin T.M., and Paul R.H.: A comparison of misoprostol and prostaglandin E2 gel for preinduction cervical ripening and labor induction. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1995; 172: pp. 1804-1810
- 35. Wing D.A., Rahall A., Jones M.M., Goodwin T.M., and Paul R.H.: Misoprostol: an effective agent for cervical ripening and labor induction. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1995; 172: pp. 1811-1816
- 36. Chuck F.J., and Huffaker B.J.: Labor induction with intravaginal misoprostol versus intracervical prostaglandin E2 gel (Prepidil gel): randomized comparison. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1995; 173: pp. 1137-1142
- 37. Stempel J.E., Prins R.P., and Dean S.: Preinduction cervical ripening: a randomized prospective comparison of the efficacy and safety of intravaginal and intracervical prostaglandin E2 gel. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1997; 176: pp. 1305-1309
- 38. Farah L.A., Sanchez-Ramos L., Rosa C., et al: Randomized trial of two doses of the prostaglandin E1 analog misoprostol for labor induction. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1997; 177: pp. 364-369
- 39. Chyu J.K., and Strassner H.T.: Prostaglandin E2 for cervical ripening: a randomized comparison of Cervidil versus Prepidil. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1997; 177: pp. 606-611
- 40. Gottschall D.S., Borgida A.F., Mihalek J.J., Sauer F., and Rodis J.F.: A randomized clinical trial comparing misoprostol with prostaglandin E2 gel for preinduction cervical ripening. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1997; 177: pp. 1067-1070
- 41. Perry K.G., Larmon J.E., May W.L., Robinette L.G., and Martin R.W.: Cervical ripening: a randomized comparison between intravaginal misoprostol and an intracervical balloon catheter combined with intravaginal dinoprostone. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1998; 178: pp. 1333-1340
- 42. Wing D.A., and Paul R.H.: Induction of labor with misoprostol for premature rupture of membranes beyond thirty-six weeks’ gestation. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1998; 179: pp. 94-99
- 43. Practice-relevant changes to the DSM-IV-TR. Clarification of the definition of pervasive developmental disorders not otherwise specified. Available at: http://www.psychiatry.org/practice/dsm/practice-relevant-changes-to-the-dsm-iv-tr. Accessed Aug. 28, 2013.
- 44. Volkmar F.R., Shaffer D., and First M.: PDDNOS in DSM-IV. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30: pp. 74-75