Phonological disorder and CAS: Characteristics, goals and treatment

Chapter 6
Phonological disorder and CAS: Characteristics, goals and treatment


Chapter 6 addresses a range of practical issues in the dynamic assessment, and differential diagnosis of children with moderate and severe SSD, including childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) or suspected CAS, sometimes abbreviated as sCAS. The chapter also contains suggestions for and discussion of intervention goals, approaches and techniques for these children. It starts with the proposition (Velleman, 2005) that phonological disorder and CAS have at least six inter-related characteristics in common that, when it gets right down to it in everyday practice, we find ourselves treating symptomatically, while still taking the primary diagnosis into account. In this sense, we ‘treat the symptoms and not the label’.


The four contributors to this chapter are Chantelle Highman (A41) on staying in touch with the juried literature, Karen Froud and Reem Khamis-Dakwar (A42) on the neural underpinnings of CAS, and Amy E. Skinder-Meredith (A43) on rating a child’s speech characteristics.


Treat the symptoms, not the label


As a motor speech disorder, CAS is a discrete diagnostic subtype of childhood (paediatric) SSD. There is a conservative consensus view that it is best characterised as a symptom complex rather than as a unitary disorder and that it may affect, to varying degrees, some combination of: non-speech motor behaviours; speech motor behaviours; production of speech sounds and structures (word and syllable shapes); prosody; language; metalinguistic/phonemic awareness; and literacy (ASHA, 2007b). Maassen (2002) reflected on a finding of Shriberg, Aram, and Kwiatkowski (1997c) that, whereas late onset of speech and slow development are usual in CAS, neither a typical phonological developmental pathway nor a characteristic phonological profile for children with sCAS has been found, and that CAS has no phonological characteristics that are uniquely its own.


It is also generally agreed that many ‘CAS characteristics’ can be found in children with other subtypes of SSD, and that CAS and phonological disorder can co-occur in an individual child. Little wonder then, that Velleman and Strand (1994) declared that this, ‘may result in a variety of motor, phonologic, linguistic or neurologic signs or symptoms and in fact inconsistency among symptoms may be expected as typical’.


Current thinking in academia, and in the clinical field, tends towards a focus on the overlap of symptoms, and the overlap of treatment methodologies, for children with CAS and children with moderate through to severe phonological disorder. A common-sense (to some) symptomatic approach to treatment has emerged. Velleman (2005) wrote: ‘CAS is different from “regular” phonological disorders, but there are still patterns to be found and treated. There is a great deal of overlap in the symptoms of CAS and the symptoms of other phonological disorders, so it is often difficult to decide whether a diagnosis of CAS is appropriate. But, in a sense, that does not matter. Treat the symptoms, not the label. But then, in an area that enjoys its spirited controversies, this idea does not appeal to all!


Six characteristics CAS and phonological disorder may have in common


The characteristics that can be evident in either disorder, or that may be present when CAS and phonological disorder co-occur in the same child, are



  1. consonant (C), vowel (V) and phonotactic inventory constraints (i.e., consonants, vowels and syllable-word shapes are missing from the respective inventories);
  2. omissions of segments and structures: that is, omissions of consonants, vowels and syllable shapes that are already present in the child’s repertoire;
  3. vowel errors including vowel replacements and vowel distortions;
  4. altered suprasegmentals: that is, atypical prosody;
  5. increased errors with utterance length and/or complexity; and,
  6. the use of simple, but not complex, syllable and word shapes.

In Box 6.1, the characteristics and signs of phonological disorder are listed in the left column and suggestions for how to test and what to look for are listed in the right column. Similarly, Box 6.2 shows the characteristics of CAS, how to test, and observations to be made.


Intervention goals that are common to phonological disorder and CAS


The six characteristics are listed in the left column of Table 6.1. Reading across Row 1, we see that the first characteristic is consonant and vowel inventory constraints, or in other words, missing vowels and consonants. In phonological disorder, this manifests as systemic simplifications (substitution processes), such as stopping, gliding and fronting where one sound replaces another (see Table 2.4 for consumer-friendly descriptions and examples these error-types and more).


Table 6.1 Phonological disorder and childhood apraxia of speech: Characteristics and goals in common












































Phonological disorder (PD) and childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) Phonological disorder Childhood apraxia of speech Phonological disorder and CAS
Characteristics in common Typical errors Typical errors Typical goals
(1) Consonant inventory constraints; vowel inventory constraints; and phonotactic inventory constraints. Simplifications in the form of systemic or substitution processes, e.g., stopping, gliding; simplifications of syllable structures, e.g., FCD, CR, WSD Simplifications AND increased segmental complexity: e.g., affricates replacing stops; clusters replacing singletons; diphthongs replacing vowels

  1. Consonant inventory expansion.
  2. Vowel inventory expansion.
  3. Phonotactic inventory expansion.
(2) Omissions of consonants, vowels and syllable shapes that are already in the inventory. Simplifications in the form of syllable structure processes and phonotactic errors: e.g., ICD, FCD, CR, WSD Simplification AND increased structural complexity, e.g., epenthesis (schwa insertion) /səked/ for ‘scared’.

  1. Syllable shape inventory expansion.
  2. Word shape inventory expansion.
  3. Increased accuracy of production of target structures.
(3) Vowel errors. Vowel errors are less common in children who don’t have CAS Vowel errors are more common, and more persistent in CAS

  1. More complete vowel repertoire.
  2. More accurate vowel production.
(4) Altered suprasegmentals. Weak syllable deletion Excessive and equal stress

  1. Production of strong and weak syllables.
  2. Differentiation of strong and weak syllables.
(5) More errors with longer and/or more complex utterances, including the so-called ‘SODA’ errors of substitution, omission, distortion and addition. SODA, process errors, and segmental complexity errors increase as contexts become more difficult, reducing intelligibility. SODA, process errors, and segmental complexity errors increase as contexts become more difficult. This is even more obvious in children with CAS.

  1. Generalisation of new consonants and vowels, syllable structures, and word structures, to more challenging contexts.
(6) Use of simple, but not complex, syllable shapes and word shapes. Syllable structure processes: ICD, FCD, CR, WSD and reduplication Syllable structure processes are more prevalent and persistent, even when phonetic repertoire is apparently adequate.

  1. More complete phonotactic repertoire.
  2. More varied use of phonotactic range within syllables and words.
  3. Improved accuracy.

In CAS, consonant and vowel inventory constraints manifest as the same sorts of simplification errors that are found in phonological disorder, but in addition to these, errors that involve increased phonetic complexity are found. For example, my client Costa, 5;8, with CAS referred to ‘Henry the green engine’ which has 10 consonants, as [ˈdʒϵndˌɹi vɜ ˈdʒɹʷind ˈϵnˌdʒɹənd] which has 15. Other examples of his ‘complexification’ were:
























jump dʒɹʌːmpə blackout ˈvəɹakˌʒout real bəɹiʊʷdə
twin tʃəɹɪnᵈ kilometer ˈklɪləˌməməˈlitlə fishing ˈtʃɪˌtʃɪŋ
fastest tʃəˈɹatˌsɪst Mrs. Oates ˈmɪtʃˌʌz ˌoutˈʃəz creepy-crawley ˈkəlibˌɹiˈɡlɔˌɹid

Costa’s complexity errors included schwa addition and insertion (both occurred in real), affricates replacing stops, for example, top pronounced as [dʒɒbə] or [dzɒptə]; clusters replacing singletons, for example, rabbit pronounced as [bɹæbɪt] or [dɹæbɪt]; and diphthongs replacing vowels, for example, bed pronounced as [baɪd] or [biːəd].


In the right column of Row 1, we see that the typical therapy goals in common are consonant and vowel inventory expansion to give the child ‘more to work with’. The same format is used in the following five rows of Table 6.1. The clinician then has to determine how best to address these goals given the child’s overall presenting picture. This question of ‘how’ is addressed towards the end of this chapter, and the reader who cannot stand the suspense is referred to Table 6.4!


A difficulty associated with this commonality of characteristics is that, when families who do not have a background in SLP/SLT seek out information without professional guidance, suspecting or even convinced that their child has CAS, they will often recognise enough ‘features’ of CAS to be certain that they ‘know’ what their child’s speech problem is. Clinical experience suggests that they often do this without realising that those same symptoms are far more likely to signal phonological disorder, given the low incidence of CAS.


Compounding this problem, when lay people turn to the Internet for elucidation, they find a disproportionately high number of websites dealing with CAS, the less frequent speech sound disorder, and these may contain inaccurate information. Some of the misleading sites present unsubstantiated opinion and supposition as fact to sell products (e.g., dietary supplements and complementary treatments) and services (e.g., online treatment). By contrast, few sites deal explicitly and accurately with phonological disorder, the more common SSD. It may not be obvious to lay people that SSD affects about 7 children in every 100, with approximately 86% of these children having articulation and phonological difficulties (if we combine SD-GEN and SD-OME), or 98% if we combine SD-GEN, SD-OME and SD-DPI, whereas the CAS-affected children (SD-AOS) represent a miniscule <1% of the SSD population (as discussed in Chapter 2, and see Shriberg et al. (2010)).


An exemplary Internet source of trustworthy CAS information for consumers and clinicians is the Apraxia-KIDS website (Gretz, A7). A positive effect of the growth of Apraxia-KIDS and CASANA has been increased accuracy of information about speech development and disorders circulating on the Web, and with it enhanced communication concerning CAS between consumers and professionals. For some clinicians, this has enabled a ‘more equal’ relationship with clients and a sharper appreciation of the effects of communication disorders on affected individuals and their families, perhaps with the added benefit of improving their skills both as counsellors, and of knowing when to refer to a professional counsellor (Bitter, A15; Overby & Bernthal, A16; Stoeckel, A40).


Among the issues and concerns that arise in parent and family counselling, and in counselling ‘older’ children and youth with persisting SSD, are the potential long-term consequences of these conditions. Gierut (1998) provides a concise summary of likely repercussions, indicating that some individuals can expect lifetime challenges in terms of their retrieval, manipulation and comprehension of linguistic information; their expressive language capabilities; and their education and work choices. Discussing these ramifications, Gierut, p. S87, takes the view that, ‘research calls for both retrospective and prospective studies of the etiology of phonological disorders and the identification of integrated causal relationships and their outcome on a speaker’s daily life’. Such implications impact the assessment and intervention process, right from the opening moments of the initial consultation or case history interview.


Case history interview


Table 6.2 provides ‘assessment prompt’ notes, developed over many years, that clinicians can use, and potentially modify, for case history-taking when they suspect that a child has CAS. Procedures and observations will vary with the child’s age and stage; there is overlap between the 10 sections, and not every section will be needed for every child. If they feel it may facilitate communication, information sharing and insight building, the clinician may wish to provide parents with the prompt, without the footnotes, prior to, or at the beginning of the case history and assessment process. This is not to suggest that it be used as a questionnaire, but rather as a general indication to parents of the territory to be covered in the process of conducting the initial assessment and working towards a provisional diagnosis or diagnosis.


Table 6.2 A 10-point CAS Assessment Prompt


















































Procedures and observations vary with the child’s age and stage. Note that there is overlap between the 10 sections.
1. Hearing Audiology report Hearing history
2. Development Perinatal history
Milestones
Motor development
Cognition
see point 4 below Psychometric/Paediatric report. Be mindful that that speakers with CAS have speech processing deficits in encoding, memory and transcoding (Shriberg, Lohmeier, Strand & Jakielski, 2012).
Social development
Wants to communicate; solitary; aloof; ‘separation issues’; clingy, why? Play: who with? Nature of play? Persistent personality? Reactive personality? Ask about teasing/bullying (Hennessy & Hennessy, 2013, pp. 119–122).

Feeding
Latching issues, fighting the breast, sucking, lactation consultant, other intervention re: feeding (and/or sleep pattern), drinking from cup, chewing, gag reflex, vomiting, reflux, failure to thrive, holding food in the mouth, breast or bottle, diet, mouth as a sensor, mouth stuffing, variety of foods.
Health/Wellbeing
Illnesses, ‘always at the doctor’s office’, accidents, injuries, ear infections, seizures, ‘separation’, hospital, operations, tires easily, sleep pattern; always ‘on the go’.
Intelligibility
Ask parents: can you put a percentage on it? Who understands? Does another child ‘interpret’? Does the other child make mistakes in interpreting? Does the child’s intelligibility vary? Worse when tired? Worse/better in certain situations? Worse with longer utterances? Uses signs or actions to help get the message across. Aware?

Babble
Quiet baby, no babbling, late babbling; lots of babbling, lots of vocal play (gurgling/ raspberries); undifferentiated babbling (all sounded the same); few or no consonants in babbled utterances; ‘babble’ mainly squeals and grunts (i.e., not true babbling).

Imitation
Tries hard to imitate all the time; Little attempt to imitate sounds; disinterested in imitating words; refusal to imitate words; upset if asked to imitate words. Does/does not imitate play.
Sounds/Words
Can say words that are not used in every day speech. How many intelligible words; how many approximations; first words (when?), low vocabulary for age (parents’ judgement), comparison with other children in the family and/or age-peers; one word for many meanings (‘big’ for all machines/vehicles). Only says words at home. Won’t attempt certain words. Can’t say own name. Plays remarkably quietly. Low volubility at home (parents’ judgement).
Gesture/Grunts
Uses gesture instead of words; uses vowels and grunts instead of words. Creative use of gesture.







Frustration
Frustrated when not understood (or passive, or unhappy), or ‘resigned’ or ‘adjusted’ to not being a talker?

Lost sounds/words
Says a word and it is never heard again, keeps a word for a while then ‘loses’ it; words come and go. Sounds come and go.

Groping/Struggle
Mouthing words? Silent posturing?
Comprehensiona
Ask parents for example of how well the child comprehends spoken language. Is there a receptive–expressive gap (comprehension higher than output would suggest)?
Theories? History?
Ask parents what they think the problem might be (or might be ‘called’). Have you wondered about a ‘label’, searched the web and joined a discussion group, received advice or ‘suggestions’ from family, friends and others? Thought about family history? What brought you here?
3. Language Ask parents; formal tests
4. Cognition Ask parents; formal tests
5. PA/Literacy Ask parents; formal tests. Story time? Consider offering ONE piece of advice.b
6. Neuromuscular examination Posture (sitting/W-sitting?)
Muscle
Tone
Coordination
Reflexes
Gait Sensory function
Involuntary movements
(if yes, query dysarthria)
Physiotherapist or OT reports?
7. Motor speech examination (Table 6.3)
8. Speech and non-speech characteristics – note the general agreement between ASHA (2007a,b) and Davis, Jakielski and Marquardt (1998)
Non-speech characteristics speech (Davis et al., 1998; see also Teverovsky, Bickel & Feldman, 2009 for parents’ perception of the functional characteristics of children with CAS within the ICF-CY framework)
(1) Impaired volitional oral movements
(2) Reduced expressive compared to receptive language skills (receptive–expressive gap)
(3) Reduced diadochokinetic rates
Speech characteristics (Davis et al., 1998)


  1. Limited consonant repertoire
  2. Limited vowel repertoire
  3. Frequent omissions
  4. High incidence of vowel errors
  5. Inconsistent articulation errors (Token-to-Token)
  6. Altered suprasegmentals (prosody)
  7. Increased errors with output length/complexity
  8. Difficulty in imitation (groping or refusal)
  9. Use of simple syllable shapes


9. Speech assessment
Standardised Articulation and Phonology Test (e.g., DEAP, HAPP-3)
Independent and Relational Analysis
Inconsistency Assessment
Compare Single Word and Conversational Speech PCC and PVC
Intelligibility Ratings
Use CS sample for MLUm and Structural Analysis if formal language testing is not possible
Look for silent posturing/groping
Is the prosodic contour of utterances/sentences intact on imitation?
Contrastive stress (I WANT one/I want ONE/I want one)
Rule a dysarthria component out or in if possible (Skinder-Meredith, A43)
Rule a phonological component out or in if possible
10. Speech characteristics rating (Table A43.1)

a ‘Parents will often say that their child ‘understands everything’ or is ‘very bright’ and many can give excellent examples of why they see their child this way, but it is essential to test receptive skills. Parent report may be positive but testing tells you about co-operation and attention too, possibly revealing subtle deficits in comprehension. This can come as a shock to parents and requires sensitive handling – don’t assume they ‘know’.


b Consider offering one piece of advice in the initial visit. At this point parents are usually not ready to absorb a lot of new information, but they will usually remember one important suggestion. Recommending a regular 5–7 minute ‘story time’ or ‘communication time’ or ‘talking time’ when a parent engages quietly with the child with books, pictures or ‘literacy-like’ activities, sends a message about the importance of both literacy and 1:1 child–adult communication opportunities, and once established can become the basis for a speech homework routine. Suggest concrete ways that this might be accomplished even with children who don’t like books, keeping the demands on the parent reasonable and practical as they adjust to the diagnosis or suspected diagnosis.


Motor speech examination worksheet


In working through the motor speech examination worksheet, displayed in Table 6.3, the tasks chosen and the order or presentation depend on the severity of the particular child’s difficulties and any predictions the clinician makes regarding his or her probable performance. The worksheet is intended to help the investigator confirm or reject CAS as a diagnosis, bearing in mind that any determination of oral apraxia would have been made during the structural–functional examination (Skinder-Meredith, A43).


Table 6.3 Motor Speech Examination Worksheet




































































This worksheet was designed by Edythe Strand, and is used by permission (Bowen, 2009, pp. 212–213). The stimuli used as examples in this version are from the DEMSS (Strand, A45).
(A) Observations during connected speech Example for a young child or one with very Severe impairment

Vowels Consonants Typical maximum word length Syllable shapes C, CV, VC MLU
Conversation



Picture description



Narrative



(B) Observations of elicited utterances Example for a young child or one with very Severe impairment Examine, dynamically, the child’s ability to sequence movement for phonetic sequences in various contexts: (1) Vowels (2) CV VC CVC (3) Monosyllabic, bisyllabic, polysyllabic words (4) Phrases (5) Sentences of increasing length looking at the child’s: Movement accuracy; Vowel production; Consistency; and Prosody, and the level of support required. You don’t have to use the DEMSS stimuli (below); use stimuli that ‘suit’ the child.

Immediate repetition Repetition after delay – no cues Simultaneous production needed Gestural/tactile cues needed
Isolated vowels


CV
me hi



VC
up eat



Reduplicated syllables
mama booboo



CVC1
mom peep pop



CVC2
mad bed hop



Vowel errors
Note the different coarticulatory contexts



Utterances of increasing length (Note the use of simple words)
Bi-syllabic 1
baby puppy



Bi-syllabic 2
bunny happy today canoe



Multi-syllabic
banana video



Phrases
Make up stimuli to suit
the child being tested
Me too;
Big boy



Sentences
Make up stimuli of increasing length to suit the child.
Dad.
Hi dad.
Hi daddy.




As with the assessment prompt (Table 6.2), the procedures in Table 6.3 overlap, and not all will be done with every client. There is no particular order of presentation of these tasks, other than the logical hierarchy that the clinician deems appropriate for the particular individual.


Multi-Syllabic words


James (A50) identified the following 10 words as being the most ‘clinically useful’ or most revealing diagnostically: ambulance, hippopotamus, computer, spaghetti, vegetables, helicopter, animals, caravan, caterpillar and butterfly, in her study of polysyllabic words and words containing consonant clusters. These words are displayed in Figure 6.1.

images

Figure 6.1 Debbie’s 10 long clinically useful words


Reading and reviewing the literature


In order to adequately inform and support families, and to fulfil the requirements of evidence-based and ethical practice (Powell, A39), we must be au fait with the relevant literature, whether by reading it diligently as a component of a personal learning plan or within a professional learning network (PLN), or by absorbing it in ‘distilled’ form at CPD/CEU events, where it is often presented by individuals pursuing, or holding, doctoral degrees and who have truly immersed themselves in a topic.


Among the first steps in the PhD process (Mewburn, 2013; Petre & Rugg, 2010) are choosing both a topic area and a research question or questions. This requires the identification of a ‘do-able’ (by one person) piece of original research, and then the development of a proposal: which is probably when the real work of the dissertation begins. Completing a focused literature review around the research topic and questions is an important aspect of this endeavour, with the review itself eventually becoming the essence of the introduction to the doctoral dissertation. In response to what is found in the literature, and in response to what emerges from discussion with advisors or supervisors (Deem & Brehony, 2000) and mentors, the potential doctoral candidate may reach a point of wanting to reformulate the topic and/or questions, abandoning some questions and honing and sharpening others. The aim of the review is to frame and shape the research, demonstrate that it can fill a crucial gap, and link it to the larger body of knowledge (Mullins & Kiley, 2002). Ultimately, according to Sternberg (1981), the literature review tells readers that the candidate has grasped the subject, has connected the topic to larger historic and current themes, can demonstrate that the proposed contribution is unique, and that the candidate can produce and critically evaluate an astutely refined and focused bibliography.


Dr. Chantelle Highman is an Australian SLP working as a clinician and researcher in Perth, Western Australia. She completed her doctoral degree in School of Psychology and Speech Pathology at Curtin University, investigating potential early speech motor and language precursors in infants at risk of CAS, and looking for evidence for a motor-specific core deficit for the disorder (Maassen, 2002). Her methodology across three studies has involved a combination of retrospective reports by parents of children with CAS, case study analyses of retrospective CAS data and a prospective longitudinal study of siblings of children with CAS, providing an insight into the potential earliest features of disordered speech motor control (Highman, Hennessey, Leitão & Piek, 2013; Highman, Hennessey, Sherwood & Leitão, 2008; Highman, Leitão, Hennessy & Piek, 2012). One driver of this research is a question often asked by parents of children with CAS regarding the ‘early warning features’ they should be watchful for in their younger children, an obvious one that most families probably think we would be able to answer quite easily.


By some quirky synchronicity, Dr. Highman completed her literature review just as the final draft of the ASHA Draft Technical Report on CAS was circulated for comment. In her response to Q41, she discusses the role and process of reading the literature as it relates to practice, and the therapy and research implications of the ratified report (ASHA, 2007a) and position statement (ASHA, 2007b).





Neurophysiological investigations


The reader has been introduced to Dr. Karen Froud in the preamble to her essay on non-linearity in the previous chapter (Froud, A38). Her co-author in A42 is Dr. Reem Khamis-Dakwar.


Dr. Khamis-Dakwar holds a PhD in speech-language pathology from Teachers College, Columbia University, and is currently working as assistant professor in the Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders at Adelphi University in Long Island, New York. She is the director of the Neurophysiology in Speech Language Pathology Lab, where she conducts research into the neural correlates of linguistic processing and representation in specific sociolinguistic situations, such as Arabic diglossia, and functional changes related to SLP treatment and language learning. She is an expert in speech-language service provision for culturally and linguistically diverse populations, especially Arabic-speaking communities.





Characteristics and general observations of CAS


Adopting the suggestions of Shriberg, Campbell, Karlsson, McSweeney and Nadler (2003), the segmental and suprasegmental characteristics the clinician will look for with CAS as a suspected, provisional or working diagnosis are listed below. Then follows a guide to the general observations an SLP/SLT might make during differential diagnosis. These are arranged under the overlapping section headings of: general characteristics, phonetic characteristics/phonetic error-types, sound sequencing difficulties, timing disturbances, disturbed temporal–spatial relationships of the articulators, contextual changes in articulatory proficiency, phonological awareness, receptive language and expressive language.


Segmental characteristics of CAS



  1. Articulatory struggle and silent posturing
  2. Transpositional substitution errors
  3. Marked inconsistency (especially token-to-token variability)
  4. Sound and syllable deletions
  5. Vowel and/or diphthong errors

Suprasegmental (prosodic) characteristics of CAS



  1. Inconsistent realisation of stress
  2. Inconsistent realisation of temporal constraints on both speech and pause events
  3. Inconsistent oral–nasal gestures underlying the percept of nasopharyngeal resonance

General observations of CAS

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jun 18, 2016 | Posted by in PEDIATRICS | Comments Off on Phonological disorder and CAS: Characteristics, goals and treatment

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access