Ambivalent/resistant (C)
The baby is distressed during separations. During the reunions, the baby both resists and seeks contact with the caregiver
C1
The baby is extremely distressed during separations. During reunions, resistant behavior (mixed with contact seeking) is especially notable. There is an angry quality to the interaction
C2
The baby appears passive both in limited exploratory behavior and failure to actively initiate interactions with caregiver. During reunions, the baby signals need for contact but resists contact
Table 6.2
Description of ambivalent/resistant attachment in preschool child-caregiver dyads. (Source: Cassidy and Marvin 1992)
Ambivalent (C) | The child seems reluctant to explore and preoccupied with parent. However, interactions are not relaxed and child seems unable to be comforted by parent. The child may try to involve the parent in interaction by stating he can’t do something but then protest or resist parent’s attempts to interact. Child protests separation from parent and may be highly distressed or angry during separation. During the reunions, the child appears angry and frustrated, continuing to alternatively seek and reject contact |
Resistant (C1) | Parent-child struggle is prominent. This may involve child wanting to engage in some activity while the parent actively discourages the activity or the child resisting the parent’s involvement in play. Struggle may escalate to the point of child hitting the parent |
Immature (C2) | The child appears immature, seeking the parent’s attention through baby talk, clinging, or following parent. The child is unable to use the parent as a secure base for exploration. The child is preoccupied with the parent but does not seem to enjoy interactions with the parent |
Research on Parenting Behaviors Associated with Ambivalent/Resistant Attachment
Caregivers in dyads with this pattern of attachment tend to respond inconsistently to their child’s attachment needs. Longer work hours, parenting stress, and placement in group day care have also been associated with ambivalent infant-mother attachment (Scher and Mayseless 2000). Mothers in dyads with ambivalent attachment report more separation anxiety than mothers of securely attached dyads (Scher and Mayseless 2000). Dyads with ambivalent/resistant attachment at 15 months have significantly more episodes of a pattern described as “chase and dodge” during face-to-face interactions at four months of age (Beebe et al. 2010). That is, when the infant looked away from the mother to modulate arousal, the mother would “chase” the child to make eye contact by moving her head towards the infant’s face. Mothers of infants who develop ambivalent/resistant attachment use more intrusive touch (scratching, pulling, pushing, poking, pinching) with their infants and, by four months of age, their infants are not emotionally organized by their mother’s touch (Beebe et al. 2010). Thus, while physical contact typically contributes to the development of a secure attachment, for young children who have experienced excessive intrusive touch from their caregivers, physical contact with their caregiver may be dysregulating. Infants with an ambivalent/resistant attachment with their caregiver cannot count on their caregiver’s availability so need to maintain constant vigilance rather than exploring. However, they are also on guard against intrusive interactions with their caregiver so are unable to relax and accept comfort from her when she is available.
It has been hypothesized that inconsistent responding in some caregivers in ambivalent/resistant dyads may result from an unconscious desire to heighten the child’s attachment behavior (Beebe et al. 2010). In other caregivers, the inconsistent responding may be the result of parenting stress (Scher and Mayseless 2000).
Outcome Studies on Attachment-Based Interventions: Rate of Ambivalent/Resistant Attachment
As shown in Tables 6.3 and 6.4, the relatively low rates of ambivalent/resistant attachment make conclusions about which attachment-based interventions are especially effective at reducing ambivalent/resistant attachment tentative. However, a variety of approaches focusing on providing parents with support and increasing parents’ sensitive responsiveness to attachment signals seem to be associated with reductions in ambivalent/resistant attachment. In families with a history of maltreatment, interventions that provide support to parents and reinforce maternal sensitive responsiveness are associated with improvements in ambivalent/resistant attachment (Cicchetti et al. 2006; Moss et al. 2011). In dyads with irritable infants, a skills-based intervention focused on improving sensitive responsiveness (van den Boom 1994) is associated with improvement in ambivalent/resistant attachment while the Circle of Security home visiting approach (Cassidy et al. 2011) is not. One of the strategies used for intrusive mother of irritable babies in van den Boom’s (1994) approach is having the mother describe what her infant is doing which may reduce the pattern of chase and dodge reported in ambivalent/resistant dyads. The Circle of Security (COS) protocol used in an at-risk Head Start group also fails to lead to a decrease in the rate of ambivalent/resistant attachment (Hoffman et al. 2006).
Table 6.3
Ambivalent/resistant attachment outcomes in intervention studies
Population | Model | Rate of ambivalent/resistant attachment | Reference |
---|---|---|---|
Infants in maltreating families (United States) N = 137 | Child-parent psychotherapy (CPP) | Pre CPP: 3 % | (Cicchetti et al. 2006) |
Post CPP: 0 % | |||
Nurse family partnership (NFP) | Pre NFP: 4 % | ||
Post NFP: 0 % | |||
Community standard care (CS) | Pre CS: 4 % | ||
Post CS: 2 % | |||
Toddlers of mothers with Postpartum Major depressive disorder (United States) N = 130 | Child-parent psychotherapy (CPP) | Pre CPP: 9 % | (Toth et al. 2006) |
Post CPP: 4 % | |||
Control (Con) | Pre Con: 9 % | ||
Post Con: 7 % | |||
At-risk head start and early head start group (United States) N = 65 | Circle of security (COS) | Pre COS: 3 % | (Hoffman et al. 2006) |
Post COS: 11 % | |||
First born, irritable infants in economically stressed households (United States) N = 220 | Circle of security—home visiting-4 intervention (COS-HV4) | COS-HV4: 15 % | (Cassidy et al. 2011) |
Control (Con) | Con: 15 % |
Table 6.4
Ambivalent/resistant attachment outcomes in parent coaching interventions
Population | Model | Rate of ambivalent/resistant attachment | Reference |
---|---|---|---|
First born, irritable infants in low socioeconomic households (Netherlands) N = 100 @ 12 mos.; 82 @ 18 mos | Skills-based intervention (SBI) control (Con) | SBI @ 12 mos.: 6 % SBI @ 18 mos.: 2 % Con @ 12 mos.: 14 % Con @ 18 mos.: 13 % | (van den Boom 1994) |
Maltreated infants and preschool-aged children (Canada) N = 67 | Relationship intervention program (RIP) community services (CS) | Pre RIP: 6 % Post RIP: 0 % Pre CS: 16 % Post CS: 9 % | (Moss et al. 2011) |
Characteristics of Preoccupied State of Mind
Characteristics of preoccupied state of mind are summarized in Table 6.5. Preoccupied state of mind occurs in 3–10 % of adolescents and adults (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. 2009; Booth-LaForce and Roisman 2014; Caspers et al. 2007) . Rates are also relatively low in parents seeking help with parenting (1–5 %) (Korfmacher et al. 1997; Routh et al. 1995). Rates are somewhat higher among patients receiving psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder (15 %) (Levy et al. 2006).